Thursday 31 August 2017

Gluten & casein free diet vs. ketogenic diet for autism: Fight!

OK, use of the word 'fight' in the title of this post looking at the study results published by Omnia El-Rashidy and colleagues [1] is a bit melodramatic but the findings do provide some rather interesting talking points when it comes to the whole 'diet and autism' thing (see here). Not least that this is, I think, the first peer-reviewed research occasion when the use of a gluten and casein-free (GFCF) diet in the context of autism has been pitted under experimental conditions against another dietary intervention of some interest, the ketogenic diet. The GFCF diet by the way, involves the removal of foods containing the proteins gluten, found in bread and other cereal-based products and casein, the primary protein found in milk and other dairy products. The ketogenic diet is not a million miles away from the GFCF diet but focuses more on the use of high fat and low carbohydrate foods (see here). Both diets have at least some experimental evidence to suggest that they may impact on autistic and related symptoms at least for some on the autism spectrum. But the evidence is not exactly strong yet [2] for any universal effect(s)...

The results of the El-Rashidy study: both diets seemed to be associated with improvements on various autism-related measures (including the ATEC) when compared with a "third group" who "received balanced nutrition and served as a control group" for 6 months. But on some measures the ketogenic diet (KD) came out on top: "ketogenic scored better results in cognition and sociability compared to GFCF diet group."

The authors rightly note that more needs to be done in this area before any sweeping generalisations are applied. So: "this study is a single center study with a small number of patients and a great deal of additional wide-scale prospective studies are however needed to confirm these results." Add to that issues such as a lack of double-blinding (a real Achilles heel when it comes to dietary intervention research in the context of autism) and there are several more investigations needed in this area; also potentially including measurement of some biological parameters to further elucidate possible mechanisms of effect (see here for one important suggestion). But don't completely disregard all of the peer-reviewed literature in this area as bunk just yet (see here). And that for some on the autism spectrum, there may truly be a diet-sensitive phenotype to examine [3]...

----------

[1] El-Rashidy O. et al. Ketogenic diet versus gluten free casein free diet in autistic children: a case-control study. Metab Brain Dis. 2017 Aug 14.

[2] Piwowarczyk A. et al. Gluten- and casein-free diet and autism spectrum disorders in children: a systematic review. Eur J Nutr. 2017 Jun 13.

[3] Whiteley P. Nutritional management of (some) autism: a case for gluten- and casein-free diets? Proc Nutr Soc. 2015 Aug;74(3):202-7.

----------

Wednesday 30 August 2017

Vitamin D supplementation did not affect depression scores in bipolar disorder

The paper by Wendy Marsh and colleagues [1] is the offered up for discussion today and the finding that: "despite a greater rise in Vitamin D levels in the VitD supplementation group, there was no significant difference reduction in depressive symptoms" in their small cohort of adults diagnosed with "DSM IV bipolar depression and Vitamin D deficiency (<30 ng/ml)."

Regular readers of this blog might know that I'm not adverse to talking about a possible relationship between the very nebulous term 'depression' and the so-called sunshine vitamin/hormone that is vitamin D (see here and see here for examples) on the basis of measured vitamin D levels typically being lower in this population. In the specific context of bipolar disorder - where moods can swing between the extremes of depression and mania - there is also some evidence that the diagnosis is by no means protective against vitamin D insufficiency/deficiency [2] and hence supplementation might be indicated where deficiency/insufficiency is detected. Could supplementation to correct a vitamin D deficiency also impact on depressive symptoms? Well, it may not be as simple as that as per other recent examples...

Marsh et al utilised the gold standard in scientific trial design (double-blind, placebo-controlled) where "5000IU [international units] Vitamin D3 po qday supplementation" was pitted against placebo for 12 weeks. As well as looking at whether vitamin D levels improved, researchers examined scores on various schedules looking at aspects of mood (depression, anxiety, mania).

Results: when it came to those mood scores, there wasn't a great deal of difference between the groups ("16 VitD vs 17 placebo subjects") after 12 weeks. The authors note that: "MADRS [Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scalescore decreased significantly in both placebo... and VitD groups... but there were no differences between treatment groups." So, it seems that vitamin D supplementation is not some sort of cure-all for aspects of bipolar depression; bearing in mind the relatively small participant group numbers included for study.

There was also another quite intriguing observation raised by the authors: "At 12wks, the placebo group VitD levels remained unchanged, while the VitD group levels increased to 28 ng/ml." Further: "both groups’ VitD levels remained deficient." Even after 12 weeks of supplementing 5000IU daily of vitamin D3 (total dose = 420,000IU?), the supplemented group as a whole were still in the insufficient range? Of course it could be that there were issues with compliance; researchers were asking participants to take tablets daily for 12 weeks - even the most observant participants might not have completed the entire regime (particularly if they did not feel/see any effects during the trial). But I also wonder if there may other important variables affecting vitamin D levels [3] that might also account for the findings?

This is not the first time [4] that vitamin D supplementation 'for depression' has fallen under controlled conditions. I do still think that science needs to do more in this area; not least on how other confounding variables might affect efficacy and specifically how genetics might show some interplay (see here for example). We currently seemed to be faced with a picture suggestive of vitamin D inadequacy being *associated* with depression but correcting said levels (as least with oral supplementation) does not seem to be all that useful when it comes to affecting presented psychological symptoms. Well, in some cases anyway [5]...

----------

[1] Marsh WK. et al. Vitamin D supplementation in bipolar depression: A double blind placebo controlled trial. J Psychiatric Res. 2017. July 22.

[2] Boerman R. et al. Prevalence of Vitamin D Deficiency in Adult Outpatients With Bipolar Disorder or Schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2016 Dec;36(6):588-592.

[3] Mazahery H. & von Hurst PR. Factors Affecting 25-Hydroxyvitamin D Concentration in Response to Vitamin D Supplementation. Nutrients. 2015;7(7):5111-5142.

[4] Kjærgaard M. et al. Effect of vitamin D supplement on depression scores in people with low levels of serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D: nested case-control study and randomised clinical trial. Br J Psychiatry. 2012 Nov;201(5):360-8.

[5] Bahrami A. et al. High Dose Vitamin D Supplementation Is Associated With a Reduction in Depression Score Among Adolescent Girls: A Nine-Week Follow-Up Study. J Diet Suppl. 2017 Jul 31:1-10.

----------

Monday 28 August 2017

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and autism (minus the psychobabble)

Quite a few years back, I talked about the idea that a diagnosis of autism or autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is seemingly not protective of other conditions/labels appearing alongside in the context of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (see here). Described as an 'anxiety disorder', PTSD is a complicated condition typically encompassing several key features: experiencing one or more traumatic events (natural disaster, victim of crime, experiencing various forms of abuse, etc) and subsequent re-experiencing of said events (flashbacks, nightmares, anxiety, etc) significantly impacting on day-to-day life.

The paper by Haruvi-Lamdan and colleagues [1] provides some further, welcomed, research interest in the relationship between PTSD and autism in the context that a "potentially unique perception of traumatic events, particularly from the social sphere" might predispose those diagnosed with autism to be more vulnerable to PTSD than other groups.

I've titled this post 'minus the psychobabble' because, unfortunately, there is still a body of thinking out there that tries to explain concepts like PTSD in terms of schools of thought such as the psychoanalytical for example. I don't want to get into the nitty-gritty of whether your 'ego balances the id' or whatnot (and how one would actually evidence such concepts), but I do want to keep autism away from psychoanalysis. I say all that in the context that Haruvi-Lamdan et al note: "While autism and trauma were often linked in psychoanalytic theory..." but go on to provide some slightly more evidence-based discussions.

Three important points are made by the authors as to how PTSD (and trauma in general) might be linked to autism.

"First, autism spectrum disorder (ASD) may serve as a vulnerability marker for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), specifically by increasing the risk for exposure to traumatic events." Although not easy to say, a diagnosis of autism or the presence of clinically-relevant autistic traits does seemingly put someone at far greater risk for experiencing various traumatic events [2]. I speak of peer-reviewed research talking about being a victim of bullying in the context of autism (see here) for example; noting that bullying is not always 'just playground shenanigans'. I would also refer you to some other factors that don't make for great dinner-table conversation in the context of autism (see here). And if you think that such factors are typically present only in childhood, think again. This all on top of the more usual likelihood of experiencing trauma that we all face.

"Second, PTSD, once it has appeared, may exacerbate certain ASD symptoms, for example, through maladaptive coping strategies and reduced help-seeking." I'm not in total agreement with the idea of 'maladaptive coping strategies' in the context of autism because science/practice does not really know enough about coping strategies (and concepts such as resilience) either in general or in the specific context of autism. I don't doubt that individuals with and without autism might have their own way of 'coping' but there is no evidence of some 'one-size-fits-all' across the entire diagnosis or population. I do agree that there may be aspects of autism that can lead to 'reduced help-seeking' as per the issue of communication and/or language use for example, and also the strength of social circles, but that's about as far as we can reliably say at this point.

"Third, there may be shared underlying mechanisms for PTSD and ASD, including neurological abnormalities associated with both disorders, as well as cognitive and behavioral mechanisms, such as increased rumination, cognitive rigidity, avoidance, anger, and aggression." Again there are some quite sweeping generalisations included in the authors writings here - 'neurological abnormalities'? - but they do raise some important points. Rumination - a deep or considered thought about something - coupled to perseveration is something that I've covered before in the context of autism (see here). It boils down to a potential heightened risk of 'not letting go' of a topic and how, in the context of trauma and PTSD, this could be something over-represented in the context of autism or the presence of autistic traits. Added to other issues such as an intolerance of uncertainty (see here) and you can perhaps see how autism and PTSD might not necessarily be miles apart. I probably didn't need to say it but will anyway, anxiety in the context of autism seems to be both rife and in many cases, is absolutely disabling (see here).

Although there may be several inter-connecting variables that potentially unite autism and PTSD, I don't want to lose sight of the heterogeneity that is present alongside both labels. Even in the context of experiencing some truly awful acts, there are disparities in whether PTSD is diagnosed in the context of autism [3] similar to the not-autism population. Diagnosis of autism and/or clinically relevant autistic features are likely to be only part of the picture here. That being said, there does seem to be grounds for quite a bit more investigation in this area, and onward, whether screening for PTSD in the context of autism should be more widespread and also examination of how effective the intervention(s) currently in place for PTSD are in the context of autism + PTSD...

----------

[1] Haruvi-Lamdan N. et al. PTSD and Autism Spectrum Disorder: Co-morbidity, Gaps in Research, and Potential Shared Mechanisms. Psychol Trauma. 2017 Jul 20.

[2] Roberts AL. et al. Association of autistic traits in adulthood with childhood abuse, interpersonal victimization, and posttraumatic stress. Child Abuse Negl. 2015 Jul;45:135-42.

[3] Brenner J. et al. Behavioral Symptoms of Reported Abuse in Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder in Inpatient Settings. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017 Jun 7.

----------

Saturday 26 August 2017

Simvastatin + risperidone for autism?

"This study provides preliminary evidence for potential therapeutic effects of simvastatin in the treatment of autism that warrants further investigations."

So said the results of the trial published by Ehsan Moazen-Zadeh and colleagues [1] looking at whether adding simvastatin - a lipid-lowering medicine - (20-40 mg/day) or placebo to a daily regime of risperidone - an antipsychotic medicine - to a cohort of "70 drug-free children aged 4 to 12 years old with diagnosis of autistic disorder" would have any effect on the primary outcome of irritability as measured by the "Aberrant Behavior Checklist-Community (ABC-C) scale irritability subscale score." A trial entry for this study can be found here (including reference "to evaluate the efficacy of L-carnosine in the treatment of autism" in the translated version for some reason?).

Anyhow, under "randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled" conditions, researchers noted something of a potential effect from the addition of simvastatin to risperidone when compared with risperidone + placebo on the group irritability subscale scores over 10 weeks of use. They conclude that further study is required in this area.

There is very little other peer-reviewed science literature in this area of the autism research landscape. I note that Peter over at Epiphany has talked about statins and autism on a couple of occasions (see here for example) and how useful they have seemed to be for his son. Albeit based on an N=1, Peter speculated that the effect of statin use for his son (Atorvastatin) was not necessarily due to its effect on lipid lowering but rather other actions. Noting that simvastatin has specifically been linked to 'cognitive dysfunction' in some isolated cases [2] I understand that other studies have talked about a more protective 'pro-cognitive' effect from such statin use under other circumstances [3]. I do wonder if there could be something more to see in this area pertinent to at least some on the autism spectrum.

----------

[1] Moazen-Zadeh E. et al. Simvastatin as an Adjunctive Therapy to Risperidone in Treatment of Autism: A Randomized, Double-Blind, Placebo-Controlled Clinical Trial. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2017 Jul 18.

[2] Suraweera C. et al. Simvastatin-induced cognitive dysfunction: two case reports. J Med Case Rep. 2016 Apr 5;10:83.

[3] Ling Q. & Tejada-Simon MV. Statins and the brain: New perspective for old drugs. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2016 Apr 3;66:80-6.

----------

Friday 25 August 2017

"low vitamin D is related to poorer cognition" but does supplementation help?

The findings reported by Alicia Goodwill & Cassandra Szoeke [1] provided some reading interest recently and the observation that "low vitamin D is related to poorer cognition" following a systematic review and meta-analysis of the peer-reviewed science literature in this area.

Looking at the cumulative data from nearly 30 studies (most observational), researchers picked up something of a potentially important trend in relation to how low levels of measured vitamin D seemed to be *linked to* both "worse cognitive performance... and cognitive decline." This follows a trend that has been touched upon before on this blog (see here for example) and onward how applicable such findings might be to the development of conditions that primarily affect cognitive processes (see here).

But just before anyone reaches for the vitamin D as some sort of nootropic of choice, there is a cautionary tale of 'evidence currently lacking' when it comes to the use of vitamin D in an effort to try and improve cognitive performance: "Vitamin D supplementation showed no significant benefit on cognition compared with control." Indeed, such a lack of evidence kinda mirrors what has been talked about in other areas of vitamin D discussion, where for example, some types/cases of depression seem to be *associated* with vitamin D deficiency/insufficiency (see here) but oral supplementation to correct said deficiency does not always lead to improvements in the presentation of symptoms [2] despite impacting on measured biological vitamin D levels.

One might speculate as the authors did that "there is likely a therapeutic age window relevant to the development of disease and therefore vitamin D therapy" as being important. I'd also put forward the idea that further investigations taking into account the genetics of vitamin D metabolism might be similarly useful, both in the context of any vitamin D-cognition link and the proposed value of supplementation for certain groups of people.

----------

[1] Goodwill AM. & Szoeke C. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of The Effect of Low Vitamin D on Cognition. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2017 Jul 31.

[2] Kjærgaard M. et al. Effect of vitamin D supplement on depression scores in people with low levels of serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D: nested case-control study and randomised clinical trial. Br J Psychiatry. 2012 Nov;201(5):360-8.

----------

Thursday 24 August 2017

"What Happens When I Can No Longer Support My Autistic Relative?"

I've kinda touched upon the subject matter examined in the paper by Renske Herrema and colleagues [1] before on this blog (see here). Detailing important results on views and concerns about the future and specifically what will happen to loved ones with autism when families are no longer able to care for or support them, the findings approach a difficult but important question. Part of that questioning surrounds the perception that social and other supports for example, are pertinent and in place to keep loved ones safe, cared for and able to deliver suitable services onward to ensuring a nurturing environment that caters to the individual's needs, wants and wishes.

Drawing on data from "120 family members of autistic adults" (or adults with autism if you prefer), authors asked about "concerns about the future for their relative" via an online survey. Several key themes emerged from their inquiry on things like concerns for individual needs not being met, the happiness of their loved one and the question of who will care for them as and when primary caregivers are not able to or are not around to care for them. These concerns were things that quite regularly featured in the minds of family members according to their online reporting.

The authors talk about the need for planning to start early - 'timely' - when it comes to ensuring that support is both available now and in the future for family members with autism. I would definitely agree with such early planning given the history of almost Herculean efforts that parents/caregivers have had to go through to ensure that their loved ones are provided the same rights as anyone else. Indeed, legacies have already evolved from such planning (see here). I do worry however that there are factors that parents and other family members seem to have to overcome in modern times; where austerity is pushing social care to breaking point (at least here in Blighty) and the availability of social support being more and more reserved for those who cannot live independently potentially at the expense of the 'look like they're managing' masses...

----------

[1] Herrema R. et al. Brief Report: What Happens When I Can No Longer Support My Autistic Relative? Worries About the Future for Family Members of Autistic Adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017 Jul 28.

----------

Wednesday 23 August 2017

Male and female autism might not be so dissimilar?

I read with some interest the paper by Joanna Mussey and colleagues [1] concluding that "either very small or no gender differences in age at diagnosis, intelligence quotient, cognitive profiles, or autism spectrum disorder symptom severity" were detected in their cohort of males (N=566) and females (N=113) diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD).

Interest because, in these days of talk about a possible specific female autism phenotype emerging (see here) in light of potential sex differences in the presentation of autism (see here), one has to be quite careful not to over-generalise too much. In much the same way that terms such as 'male brains' and 'female brains' don't really do justice to the complexity of brain structure and function (see here), so one perhaps has to ensure that female autism and male autism presentation aren't offered in an 'either or' fashion despite the possibility of subtle differences in either the expression of symptoms or for example, differences in the presentation of things like over-represented comorbidity between the sexes/genders (see here).

Mussey et al reported that previous studies talking about autism 'hitting harder' when it comes to females in relation to the presence of intellectual (learning) disability and "more severe impairments" might not necessarily tell the full story. Based on the use of various instruments to to ascertain autistic traits and beyond, the authors noted some differences between the genders on measures of autism signs and symptoms but the precise significance was, in the most part, deemed "of minimal clinical significance." Such findings have also been noted in other recent research [2]. Another important message from the Mussey study results was that at least some of the instruments used to assess for autism or ASD might need some further investigation with sex/gender in mind.

Having said all that I do think there are still important issues that need to be further investigated when it comes to sex differences in autism presentation. The idea for example, of a 'female camouflage effect' (see here) still looms large in this area; where for example, verbal and non-verbal communication skills might serve to *mask* other important diagnostic features. I'm also [carefully] inclined to inquire whether gender identity over biological sex might play something of an important role in the male and female presentation of autism for some, in light of other important research (see here). There is a further scheme of work to be completed minus sweeping generalisations.

----------

[1] Mussey JL. et al. Are males and females with autism spectrum disorder more similar than we thought? Autism. 2017 Aug;21(6):733-737.

[2] Fulton AA. et al. Gender comparisons in children with ASD entering early intervention. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2017. 68: 27-34.

----------

Tuesday 22 August 2017

8 in 1000 babies born with foetal alcohol spectrum disorder

The results of the systematic review and meta-analysis published by Shannon Lange and colleagues [1] make for important reading with their conclusion that the global prevalence of foetal alcohol spectrum disorder (FASD) is estimated to be around 8 in 1000 live births. The accompanying editorial by Albert Chudley [2] rightly talks about 'high time for action' on the basis of such figures, particularly because FASD is a largely preventable condition and specific groups of people and specific geographical regions seem to carry elevated risk(s) when it comes to offspring FASD and so can be targeted for additional support.

Looking at the collected data from 24 studies examining the prevalence of FASD - a condition whereby babies exposed to alcohol in the womb present with specific morphological features and accompanying behavioural / cognitive issues - researchers concluded that around 7.7 per 1000 live births presented with FASD. They reported that 76 counties (of 187 countries providing data) showed an estimated prevalence rate for offspring FASD above 1% of total births. South Africa came top with over 1 in 10 births being estimated to present with FASD. Croatia, Ireland, Italy and Belarus filled the other top 5 hotspots for FASD with estimated prevalence rates between 3-5% of births derived from various methods.

'Special populations' are also mentioned in the Lange paper. Specifically: "the prevalence of FASD among special populations was 15.6 to 24.6 times higher among aboriginal populations... 5.2 to 67.7 times higher among children in care... 30.3 times higher in a correctional population... 23.7 times higher in a population with low socioeconomic status... and 18.5 times higher among a population in psychiatric care compared with the global prevalence among children and youth in the general population." One might argue that at least for some of these at-risk populations, issues such as binge drinking habits combined with an elevated risk of unplanned pregnancy [3] could lie at the root of the estimates detailed by Lange et al. I say that minus any sweeping generalisation but in light of the scientific 'facts' detailed in the latest review paper.

I note that some of the media around the Lange findings (see here) have already mentioned about some of the behavioural manifestations of FASD 'crossing over' with diagnoses such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism. I would perhaps draw your attention to a previous blogging occasion when I discussed some of the research specifically looking at any overlap between autism and FASD (see here) and how one needs to be quite careful not to overplay any would-be links despite some data on possible overlap [4] (this paper was also from Lange).

Then to the specifics of the hows-and-whys of alcohol exposure in-utero causing FASD and whether effects might somehow be mitigated. Alcohol (ethanol) is a known teratogen [5] so effects are likely to be numerous in terms of how it affects the developing foetus. I note that the word 'epigenetics' has entered the vocabulary when it comes to possible mechanisms of effect(s) and that would suggest that at some point there may be ways and means of mitigating the effects of alcohol consumption at critical periods during pregnancy. At some point.

For now the message from Lange and colleagues is clear: FASD is not an uncommon finding in many different countries and different populations, and remains largely preventable. Abstinence from alcohol is advised before and during that special time (see here). And just in case anyone thinks I'm only zooming in on mums-to-be, dads might also heed similar advice at critical periods too.

Straight Edge from Minor Threat seems an appropriate song to conclude this post given what the song title means (veganism and the non-use of prescription medicines are optional extras I might add)...

----------

[1] Lange S. et al. Global Prevalence of Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder Among Children and Youth. JAMA Pediatrics. 2017. Aug 21.

[2] Chudley AE. Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder—High Rates, High Needs, High Time for Action. JAMA Pediatrics. 2017. Aug 21.

[3] Font-Ribera L. et al. Socioeconomic Inequalities in Unintended Pregnancy and Abortion Decision. Journal of Urban Health. 2008;85(1):125-135.

[4] Lange S. et al. Prevalence of Externalizing Disorders and Autism Spectrum Disorder among Children with Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder: Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Biochem Cell Biol. 2017 May 18.

[5] Randall CL. Alcohol as a teratogen: a decade of research in review. Alcohol Alcohol Suppl. 1987;1:125-32.

----------

Monday 21 August 2017

The links between autism and ADHD: sibling study adds a new layer

'A diagnosis of autism or autism spectrum disorder (ASD) does not typically appear in a diagnostic vacuum'. I've said that sentence several times on this blog (see here for example) in line with how science has shown that many different labels (both behavioural and somatic) are over-represented when it comes to a diagnosis of autism. All very ESSENCE like (see here)...

Of the various over-represented comorbidity, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) - either in symptoms or in diagnosis - is one of the more common ones (see here); something that has implications for screening (see here) and also management. The findings reported by Yi-Ling Chien and colleagues [1] (open-access) add something to the research looking at the possible hows-and-whys of ADHD appearing alongside autism with their focus on "unaffected siblings of probands with autism and Asperger syndrome (AS)." Such work ties into that observing 'unaffected by autism' does not necessarily mean 'symptom or trait-free' in the context of ideas such as the broader autism phenotype (BAP) (see here).

With the aim to "investigate the ADHD-related traits and attention performance in unaffected siblings of probands with autism and Asperger syndrome (AS), as well as the clinical correlates of ADHD-related traits" researchers concluded that generally, unaffected siblings (unaffected by autism) of those diagnosed with an ASD "were more hyperactive/impulsive and oppositional" than those so-called typically developing controls. The finding was based on the use of various questionnaires/schedules pertinent to both the "the core symptoms of DSM-IV ADHD" and also tests to "assess attention performance."

Of particular note was the observation of "more ADHD and oppositional traits in unaffected siblings of AS probands" when looking at subgroups on the autism spectrum. With caution, the authors suggest that such a finding may be evidence "that these traits might be a broader phenotype for AS." They also posit that "more severe ADHD-related symptoms in AS probands rather than autism probands suggest that these two subtypes may not be the same in their clinical expression regarding ADHD symptoms." In these days of plural autisms (see here), things seemingly get even more complicated when diagnostic subgroup x comorbidity is also thrown into the mix.

Although quite a bit more investigation is required in this area, there is at least one important point to take from the Chien work: unaffected siblings of those diagnosed with autism - particularly Asperger syndrome - may benefit from preferential clinical assessment for something like ADHD. I say that with the understanding that a diagnosis of ADHD has been linked to a heightened risk of various 'adverse' outcomes in the longer term (see here and see here) and again, minus any sweeping generalisations, specific interventions for ADHD can seemingly mitigate quite a bit of that excess risk (see here) and onward improve quality of life and more.

----------

[1] Chien Y-L. et al. ADHD-related symptoms and attention profiles in the unaffected siblings of probands with autism spectrum disorder: focus on the subtypes of autism and Asperger’s disorder. Molecular Autism. 2017; 8: 3.

----------

Saturday 19 August 2017

Omega-3 fatty acids and ADHD meta-analysed (again)

"In summary, there is evidence that n-3 PUFAs [polyunsaturated fatty acids] supplementation monotherapy improves clinical symptoms and cognitive performances in children and adolescents with ADHD [attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder], and that these youth have a deficiency in n-3 PUFAs levels."

So said the results of the systematic review and meta-analysis published by JanePei-Chen Chang and colleagues [1] taking in the collected peer-reviewed research literature on the topic of fatty acids and ADHD. This continues a research theme down the years suggesting that said compounds might be beneficial for at least some people diagnosed with ADHD (see here) and screening for signs of omega-3 fatty acid deficiency could be preferentially clinically indicated for those diagnosed or at risk of a diagnosis.

I don't want to dwell too much on the results because (a) they speak for themselves and (b) this is an area of science that has been a talking point for quite a few years. I know there has been a degree of 'over-hype' associated with fatty acids down the ages but as part of a larger scheme of work suggesting that food and nutrition are not so detached from some behavioural/developmental diagnoses (see here for another example) I'm minded to suggest that they are given their due credit. Certainly fatty acid supplements are quite inexpensive and also seemingly useful for various aspects of physical health too.

As to the mode of effect, well, we don't know all there is to know just yet. I note that some of the authors on the Chang paper are not adverse to the idea that something like psychiatry and immune functions are linked (see here). Whether at least some cases of ADHD might be accompanied by more 'inflammatory' issues is still the source of some debate; although I'd be quick to add in the quite voluminous research suggesting that allergy and ADHD might have more than a passing relationship (see here). Is is possible that supplementation with specific types of fatty acids typically labelled as 'anti-inflammatory' [2] could be working as anti-inflammatory agents? Well, possibly, but I daresay there may be other biological processes at work too. More research is indicated but the Chang results provide yet more [strong] evidence that at least some of those with ADHD may benefit from a fish oil or two a day.

And whilst on the topic of fatty acids, I might also direct you to an interesting piece of research recently published by Sheppard and colleagues [3]...

----------

[1] Chang JC. et al. Omega-3 Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids in Youths with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Clinical Trials and Biological Studies. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017 Jul 25.

[2] Wall R. et al. Fatty acids from fish: the anti-inflammatory potential of long-chain omega-3 fatty acids. Nutr Rev. 2010 May;68(5):280-9.

[3] Sheppard KW. et al. Effect of Omega-3 and -6 Supplementation on Language in Preterm Toddlers Exhibiting Autism Spectrum Disorder Symptoms. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017. July 26.

----------

Thursday 10 August 2017

ADHD and law enforcement contact: not a straight-forward relationship

Several times on this blog I've talked about how a diagnosis of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) seems to place the recipient at quite a bit of excess risk for various unfavourable outcomes (see here). I've tried hard not to make too many sweeping generalisations on this issue; mindful that behind every statistic in every peer-reviewed bit of science there are real people and real lives. But the data is becoming quite compelling on this matter...

The paper by Mark van der Maas and colleagues [1] makes an important contribution to the idea that the relationship between a diagnosis of ADHD and contact with law enforcement 'systems' is perhaps not as straight-forward as many might believe. Concluding that: "The observed connection between ADHD and criminality may be better understood through their shared relationships with indicators of poor social bonds", researchers suggest that social factors may very well come into play.

OK, based on a sample of over 5300 adults "representative of the general population of Ontario, Canada" researchers asked participants about their "self-reported arrest on criminal charges" history alongside examining ADHD-linked symptoms via the Adult Self Report Scale (ASRS-v1.1). They also interviewed/questioned about various social bonds - household size, education level, drug and substance abuse, etc.

They observed that: "screening positive for ADHD was twice as likely... and past use of medications for ADHD three times as likely... to be associated with ever having been arrested." But... when statistical modelling took into account the data on social bonds, things started to get a little more fuzzy. So: "In the best fitting statistical model, ever having been arrested was not associated with ADHD, but it was significantly associated with indicators of strong and weak social bonds." So things like anti-social behaviour, not progressing well in educational terms and substance use (abuse) might have some important influences on contact with law enforcement agencies. A shocker, I know.

I do have to point out a few important things about this research before anyone gets too immersed in the idea that ADHD is completely off the hook. First was the reliance on self-report when it comes to both ADHD signs and symptoms and also arrest record. The ASRS might very well be a nice rough-and-ready measure of ADHD symptoms but it is no substitute for a thorough assessment for a diagnosis of ADHD. Similarly, people may not always be completely truthful when it comes to their arrest record under several circumstances including research conditions...

Second is the concept of cause-and-effect. As easy as it is to say that ADHD was not itself linked to arrest record(s), it is important not to interpret the findings to say that there is 'no connection' between ADHD and 'having been arrested'. Minus sweeping generalisations, facets of ADHD - such as impulsivity and inattention - can and do perhaps account for some of the heightened risk for various types of offending behaviour [2]. It's fine to say that these facets of ADHD might be exacerbated under conditions of substance use/abuse for example, but one could easily then ask whether ADHD might have actually been involved in facilitating such substance use/abuse in the first place. Certainly, there is (peer-reviewed) evidence that a diagnosis of ADHD - if left untreated - may very well impact on educational outcomes for example [3] which could be one of several factors in determining other life choices.

The idea however that ADHD as a sole risk factor for adverse outcomes such as law enforcement contact does not exist in some sort of social vacuum is an important one to come from data such as that presented by van der Maas et al. It is perhaps the issue of 'vulnerability' that comes to the forefront, and how an ADHD diagnosis should perhaps be explored with that tenet in mind...

----------

[1] van der Maas M. et al. Examining the effect of social bonds on the relationship between ADHD and past arrest in a representative sample of adults. Crim Behav Ment Health. 2017 Jul 5.

[2] Berryessa CM. Attention, reward, and inhibition: symptomatic features of ADHD and issues for offenders in the criminal justice system. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2017 Mar;9(1):5-10.

[3] Lu Y. et al. Association Between Medication Use and Performance on Higher Education Entrance Tests in Individuals With Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017 Jun 28.

----------

Wednesday 9 August 2017

Methylphenidate + fatty acids for ADHD? Erm, we need more science...

I don't mind telling you that I was left a bit baffled by the results published by Mahbobeh Firouzkouhi Moghaddam and colleagues [1] (open-access available here) talking about the use of specific polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs) as a sort of add-on to more traditional pharmacotherapy indicated for cases of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Baffled because science is supposed to be presented in such a way that methods and results are easy to read and intepret and hence replicate, and appropriate conclusions are supposed to be based on those results. Read on and I hope you'll see what I mean...

Based on the use of a randomised, placebo-controlled trial design, some 40 children (6-12 years of age) who obtained "the least score in an ADHD rating scale questionnaire, responding to the treatment based on least reduction of 25% of symptoms relative to the base state in ADHD scaling" were allocated to either a methylphenidate (MPH) + PUFA ("capsules containing 180mg EPA and 120 mg DHA") group or a MPH + placebo group. Participants were monitored quite regularly over 8 weeks of intervention via the ADHD rating scale adopted and "filled by the resident of psychiatry for patients of both groups." I say all that bearing in mind that I'm not exactly sure what specific ADHD rating scale was actually used during the study.

No mind, the results: "mean severity of symptoms before treatment in both groups of methylphenidate plus PUFA and placebo was the same, and severity of symptoms after treatment in the group under methylphenidate plus PUFA treatment had reduced much more compared to the placebo group, and major changes were observed in the subscale of predominantly attention deficit type." The authors were able to describe the types of symptom patterns presenting by participants in relation to ADHD type. Indeed, quite impressively: "Response to treatment (a reduction of at least 25% in the signs) in the group taking methylphenidate plus PUFA was 90% (18 patients) and in methylphenidate plus placebo group, it was 60% (12 patients)." They did also note side-effects in both groups; the most common in both groups (taking MPH) were sleep disorders and anorexia. For the PUFA group "just one case of burping" was recorded. Researchers concluded that further trials are needed to confirm/refute their findings.

Appreciating that these study results are presented in English but English is perhaps not the mother tongue of researchers, I can get past the slightly odd tone of the article text in places. I do still have an issue with not being able to find out which ADHD scale was used during the study; something that is important if someone wanted to try and independently replicate this study.

More than that, I have to say that I am also a little hesitant when it comes to the way the statistics and findings have been presented in this paper. My first reading of the results was that MPH + PUFA supplementation was superior to MPH + placebo based on the text presented in the article. A more detailed look at the findings revealed that this was not necessarily the case based on (a) looking at the comparisons across the various types of ADHD pre- and post-intervention groups (see Table 1 of the paper) and (b) comparing 'mean severity of symptoms after treatment' between the PUFA and placebo groups (Table 2). You will see that assuming a drop in ADHD scores denotes improvement in behaviour(s), Table 1 suggests that only those with the mixed/combined type of ADHD as a group showed a reduction in scores (9.4±8.39 vs. 0.6±1.20) between pre- and post-intervention with MPH + PUFAs. This was compared with two groups in the MPH + placebo arm of the trial: predominantly attention deficit and mixed type, where a reduction was noted. In all other scenarios, the group values actually increased. Bearing in mind the authors don't actually tell us how many people were included in those ADHD groups, I was a little surprised to see that the p-values remained highly significant for all ADHD types across both study arms. One can only deduce from these findings - those presented in Table 1 - that MPH + PUFA supplementation is at best, as good as MPH + placebo for a specific type of ADHD but at worst, potentially making MPH less effective in other types of ADHD.

Then to the data showing "comparison between the mean severity of symptoms in the intervention and control groups after the treatment" (Table 2). Here again, the picture is one of no real [statistical] difference between MPH alone and MPH + PUFA supplementation after 8 weeks of intervention based on the group scores and the p-values produced. Yes, you could say that MPH + PUFA seems to show some equivalence to MPH + placebo, but then the question 'why take a PUFA supplement?' comes to the surface. And please also, none of that 'almost significant' stuff based on a p-value of 0.18 for example particularly in light of other discussions...

"This study shows that PUFA is an efficient nutrient to treat ADHD and it can be used to treat patients." I'm not so sure that this sentence is completely compatible with the study findings as they are presented; both in the text of the results and the table data. Don't get me wrong, more than most I would love to see fatty acids finding their place with at least some people presenting either with a diagnosis of ADHD or significant features of ADHD (see here and see here for examples) also on the back of some very recent peer-reviewed findings [2]. The trouble is that I don't think these are necessarily the results to show that...

----------

[1] Moghaddam MF. et al. Effectiveness of methylphenidate and PUFA for the treatment of patients with ADHD: A double-blinded randomized clinical trial. Electron Physician. 2017 May 25;9(5):4412-4418.

[2] Chang JC. et al. Omega-3 Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids in Youths with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Clinical Trials and Biological Studies. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017 Jul 25.

----------

Tuesday 8 August 2017

Toxoplasma infection may be a risk factor for manifestation of psychotic-like symptoms

It's been a while since I last talked about Toxoplasma gondii on this blog (see here). All the initial research excitement from a few years back on how T. gondii exposure might correlate with various psychiatric symptoms/conditions seems to have been toned down in recent times. Still, I continue to be fascinated with the idea that at least for some, exposure to T. gondii might be more than just a somatic thing...

The findings reported by Lindgren and colleagues [1] illustrate how my interest in the gondii remains justified as per their conclusion: "Toxoplasma infection may be a risk factor for manifestation of psychotic-like symptoms." Based on data from Finland - "Health 2000, a large cross-sectional health survey of the Finnish general population aged 30 and above" - the authorship team included on the Lindgren paper contain some names familiar to the T. gondii research scene: namely Faith Dickerson and Robert Yolken.

So, the presence of lifetime psychotic-like symptoms via "section G of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview, Munich version (M-CIDI)" were analysed alongside seropositivity to Toxoplasma "defined as a cutoff of 50IU/ml of IgG antibodies" in nearly 6000 participants. Various other potentially interfering variables - "age, gender, education, region of residence, cat ownership, and C-reactive protein measuring inflammation" - were also thrown into the statistical mix. Cat ownership by the way, refers to the [disputed] research hypothesis that some cats might be unwitting hosts for the gondii with potential onward implications for owners (see here).

Results: "T. gondii seropositivity was significantly associated with clinically relevant psychotic-like symptoms... and with the number of psychotic-like symptoms." That being said, presenting with an immune profile suggestive of some contact with T. gondii did not show any significant connection with diagnosed conditions with a psychotic element to them such as schizophrenia. The authors however, felt confident enough to say that psychotic symptoms might not be totally unrelated to T. gondii exposure.

Accepting that there may be various reasons why someone might present with clinically-relevant psychotic-like symptoms [2] I find good reason to continue with the research agenda looking at T. gondii exposure and human behaviour on the basis of results such as those from Lindgren. The spectrum of labels - behavioural/psychiatric - potentially *associated* with T. gondii exposure is not to be sniffed at [3]. Even if only a proportion of cases are found to be *associated* with gondii exposure, there are treatments readily available (see here) to manage the infection. Whether such intervention(s) might also provide some relief of psychiatric symptoms alongside, is something too that needs more investigation...

----------

[1] Lindgren M. et al. The association between toxoplasma and the psychosis continuum in a general population setting. Schizophr Res. 2017 Jul 12. pii: S0920-9964(17)30391-2.

[2] Cosgrave J. et al. The interaction between subclinical psychotic experiences, insomnia and objective measures of sleep. Schizophr Res. 2017 Jul 12. pii: S0920-9964(17)30397-3.

[3] de Barros JL. et al. Is there any association between Toxoplasma gondii infection and bipolar disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2017 Feb;209:59-65.

----------

Monday 7 August 2017

Not everyone referred for an autism assessment will 'meet diagnostic criteria'

I want to introduce the findings reported by Isaac Smith and colleagues [1] to you today, and some important points related to the assessment and diagnosis of autism or autism spectrum disorder (ASD).

Looking at a cohort of youth "referred for psychological evaluations at an outpatient clinic", some 70 young adults were categorised according to their referral status (for autism or not) and outcome status (autistic or not). Authors reported on a few important things:

"Approximately half of cases referred for suspected ASD did not meet diagnostic criteria." Yes, the final numbers were quite small but this is an important finding. Minus too many sweeping generalisations, I find myself looking at that sentence again in the context of some recent social media discussions about self-diagnosis and autism (see here). Once again, minus important issues such as identity, emotions and/or politics, I find evidence that there is no substitute for a thorough professional assessment when autism is suspected. Yes, there is always the fear that self-observations might not necessarily be accurate ones [2] but...

Then: "Youth neither referred for nor diagnosed with ASD demonstrated lower anxiety than those who were referred and diagnosed." On the basis of that last sentence acknowledging that anxiety is often a frequent issue associated with autism (see here) I am, yet again, wondering whether we've been too harsh on the writings of people such as Mildred Creak and colleagues [2] for example, and their examination of the cross-over between autism and 'schizophrenic syndrome in childhood'. No, I'm not saying that autism is schizophrenia or vice-versa (despite the potential for some overlap), but their '9 key features' seems to cover quite a bit more of the essence of autism than perhaps other widely used triadic/dyadic descriptions including: e.g. "abnormal perceptual experience... acute, excessive and seemingly illogical anxiety... distortion in motility patterns." That last point on 'motility patterns' adds to the interest in how movement issues and allied presentations might also be a core feature of autism (see here).

Finally: "Comorbidity was high in all groups, including those referred primarily for ASD assessment, underscoring the importance of comprehensive assessment regardless of specificity of the referral." Comorbidity, whether behavioural/psychiatric or somatic, is a key feature of autism. The days of autism independently existing in some sort of diagnostic vacuum are becoming a distant memory (see here). More than that, the Smith findings in this area provide some exquisite evidence for the concept of ESSENCE - Early Symptomatic Syndromes Eliciting Neurodevelopmental Clinical Examinations - championed by the likes of Prof Gillberg and colleagues. The idea being that even with the label of autism outside of the equation, other developmental/behavioural/psychiatric labels/issues will often [variably] overlap (see here).

----------

[1] Smith IC. et al. The Under- and Over-Identification of Autism: Factors Associated With Diagnostic Referral. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2017 Jul 17:1-7.

[2] Lewis LF. A Mixed Methods Study of Barriers to Formal Diagnosis of Autism Spectrum Disorder in Adults. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017 Aug;47(8):2410-2424.

[3] Evans B. How autism became autism: The radical transformation of a central concept of child development in Britain. History of the Human Sciences. 2013;26(3):3-31.

----------

Saturday 5 August 2017

Pre-pregnancy weight and risk of offspring ADHD

"Compared to normal weight mothers, the risk of having a child with ADHD [attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder] was significantly increased if the mother was overweight..., obese... or severely obese."

So said the findings reported by Andersen and colleagues [1] examining data from some 80,000 mother and child pairs "participating in the Danish National Birth Cohort (DNBC)." The name of the research game was to look-see whether maternal weight before pregnancy might be an important factor when it comes to offspring developmental and behavioural outcomes specifically with ADHD and/or autism in mind.

The results showing a possible relationship between maternal pre-pregnancy weight and ADHD were to some extent mirrored in relation to offspring autism albeit not showing the 'dose trend' of hazard ratios - overweight, obese, severely obese - noted in relation  to ADHD. Indeed authors noted that: "Regarding ASD [autism spectrum disorder], an increased risk was observed in underweight... and obese... mothers" illustrating a less confident pattern of possible association.

This is not the first time that this topic has been discussed on this blog (see here). One still has to be a little careful with such 'observational' research so as not to assume that only pre-pregnancy maternal weight is a risk factor for offspring ADHD or anything else. I'm also minded to suggest that the continued use of the body mass index (BMI) statistic is not without difficulties. That and the fact that we're also no further forward when it comes to talking about possible mechanisms behind any association given the myriad of effects that excess weight can potentially cause to mother and any children to be (see here).

What we can however take from this and other independent data [2] is that the physical health of mothers (and fathers) might be quite important to offspring even when the prospect of children is still a twinkle in the eyes of parents. We're all constantly being told to eat well, exercise regularly and cut out or cut back on certain things to achieve optimal health and wellbeing. This and other research if true, suggest that heeding such advice might have inter-generational implications too, for lots of different reasons [3]...

Music to close, and sorry Your Majesty, but some of my brood find the alternative 'God Save the Queen' much more entertaining than the original...

----------

[1] Andersen CH. et al. Maternal body mass index before pregnancy as a risk factor for ADHD and autism in children. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017 Jul 15.

[2] Van Lieshout RJ. et al. Pre-pregnancy and pregnancy obesity and neurodevelopmental outcomes in offspring: a systematic review. Obes Rev. 2011 May;12(5):e548-59.

[3] Jensen ET. et al. The Relationship of Maternal Prepregnancy Body Mass Index and Pregnancy Weight Gain to Neurocognitive Function at Age 10 Years among Children Born Extremely Preterm. J Pediatrics. 2017. March 21.

----------

Friday 4 August 2017

Is current guidance for the management of CFS/ME in England fit for purpose?

"This multi-centre study in the NHS has shown that CFS/ME [chronic fatigue syndrome/myalgic encephalomyelitis] is a long term condition that persists for the majority of adult patients even after receiving specialist treatment."

So said the study results published by Simon Collin & Esther Crawley [1] (open-access) providing important information on some 400 patients diagnosed with CFS/ME who were in receipt of "NHS specialist CFS/ME services in England." Said specialist services, we are told, follow "guidance provided by the National Institute for Health & Care Excellence (NICE), including specific guidelines for diagnosis, specialist care, and ongoing management, with an overall patient-centred approach to treatment."

In case you've not followed the link to the NICE guidance covering CFS/ME, specialist care here in England generally describes various elements including the use of cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) and/or graded exercise therapy (GET) based on the PACE trial results [2] and findings from other studies. Yes, the same PACE trial that 'continues to give' when it comes to arguments/debates (delete as appropriate) in CFS/ME circles (see here); see also the end of this post for more links to more recent happenings.

The Collin/Crawley research looked at several groups - newly referred and former patients - who attended one of eleven CFS/ME specialist services and who completed various questionnaires concerning the presentation  of fatigue, physical function and other aspects like mood and pain. Importantly: "They were also asked “Do you think that you are still suffering from CFS/ME?”" at follow-up either some 1 year after their initial clinical assessment or in some cases, 2-5 years later.

Results: "50–65% experienced little or no change in their condition 1–5 years after accessing a specialist service and 10–20% reported a deterioration, up to 30% of patients reported very much or much better health (and the majority of those who experienced little or no change had improved slightly)."

There are a few ways you could look at such results. You could say that specialist CFS/ME services are doing a fairly good job for approximately a third of patients who use them based on the figures reported. The authors do note some variability between the services that were included for analysis and given also that CFS/ME probably includes various 'sub-groups' under its banner, I'm not surprised therefore that there were potential best-responders and non-responders to the intervention(s) put in place.

The other way you could look at these data is with slightly less enthusiasm. Upwards of about two-thirds of patients surveyed saw no real difference to their symptoms long-term even after accessing such specialist services. More worryingly, a significant minority ("10-20%") reported deterioration; assuming this deterioration was in part or whole linked to their receipt of said specialist services, this is potentially in direct contravention of the tenet: first do no harm. These percentages - cumulatively up to 70%+ of patients - suggesting that specialist treatment might not necessarily be fit for purpose, need also to be analysed in the context of the costs associated with such specialist provisions in these days of rationing in many other parts of the NHS (see here for an example).

There is another detail included in the Collin/Crawley paper that requires comment: "Comparison with previous literature." The authors specifically focus in on the results for CBT and GET described in the PACE trial. They note: "The proportions of PACE trial participants (in the CBT and GET arms) who rated their overall health as very much or much better at 12 and 31 months were 41% and 42–48%, respectively, compared with 28% and 30% in our study (at 12 months and 2–5 years)." The authors suggest that differences in the number/intensity of sessions provided in the PACE trial and in 'real-life' might be one variable to account for the disparity reported on but I think we have to be a little bit careful here before saying specialist services either 'aren't doing it right' or 'aren't providing enough of a service'. There is for example, a debate currently rumbling on whether the recovery statistics subsequently produced from the PACE trial were entirely representative of the data produced (see here and see here for discussion on the authors reply). Minus any sweeping generalisations but in that context, I think one has to consider that there may be simpler answers to the disparity between studies: in a real-life context, CBT and GET are not as good an intervention for CFS/ME as some people might think. Indeed, it's potentially relevant that the US CDC has omitted CBT and GET from their current guidance for CFS/ME (see here) on top of similar changes to other guidance on this topic (see here). I know there may be some push-back to the idea that CBT and GET may not be the most suitable methods for 'treating' CFS/ME but nonetheless...

The Collin/Crawley paper provides a good snapshot of what patients themselves are saying about the current methods of treating/managing CFS/ME here in England. The picture the data paints is one of quite a bit more to do when it comes to how we care for some of the most ill people in society (see here for the quality of life comparisons). The focus on CBT for example, and the idea that those with the condition perhaps just need 'a change of attitude' towards their illness, is seemingly not working as well as some might wish. Although GET has shown some signs of efficacy for some with CFS/ME (see here), the sweeping generalisation that such an intervention is suitable for all with CFS/ME is also not borne out by the data and indeed, could potentially be quite damaging to some.  And patients are now saying that in the peer-reviewed science arena as well as on other forums too. Indeed, I again draw your attention to a quote from the opinion piece by Jonathan Edwards [3]: "If they [people with CFS/ME] are still ill, presumably these approaches have failed and the priority is to find something more effective." Who would disagree with such a sentiment?

To close, and related to today's post, has the dust settled yet regarding that special issue about the PACE trial published in the Journal of Health Psychology? I would say not...

----------

[1] Collin SM. & Crawley E. Specialist treatment of chronic fatigue syndrome/ME: a cohort study among adult patients in England. BMC Health Services Research. 2017; 17: 488.

[2] White PD. et al. Comparison of adaptive pacing therapy, cognitive behaviour therapy, graded exercise therapy, and specialist medical care for chronic fatigue syndrome (PACE): a randomised trial. Lancet. 2011 Mar 5;377(9768):823-36.

[3] Edwards J. PACE team response shows a disregard for the principles of science. 2017. J Health Psychology. March 28.

----------

Thursday 3 August 2017

The pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatment of paediatric ADHD

The findings - "systematic review with network meta-analyses" - reported by Ferrán Catalá-López and colleagues [1] (open-access available here) on the topic of treating attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were expected [2].

Looking at the available peer-reviewed science comparing "the efficacy and safety of pharmacological, psychological and complementary and alternative medicine interventions for the treatment of ADHD in children and adolescents" authors identified nearly 200 randomised trials looking at various intervention options. With data from over 26,000 people diagnosed with ADHD to examine, they applied some nifty statistical analyses leading to various conclusions on the basis of their categorisations of the various interventions analysed: "pharmacological (stimulants, non-stimulants, antidepressants, antipsychotics, and other unlicensed drugs), psychological (behavioural, cognitive training and neurofeedback) and complementary and alternative medicine (dietary therapy, fatty acids, amino acids, minerals, herbal therapy, homeopathy, and physical activity)."

First: "behavioural therapy (alone or in combination with stimulants), stimulants, and non-stimulant seemed significantly more efficacious than placebo." I don't think this is a particularly novel finding given the intervention options typically indicated for ADHD (see here).

Then: "Behavioural therapy in combination with stimulants seemed superior to stimulants or non-stimulants." This is important insofar as any notion that use of pharmacotherapy alone is going to 'tackle' ADHD. It also suggests that parents, teachers and significant others have a role to play in managing the symptoms of childhood ADHD as per various examples (see here).

Also: "Most of the efficacious pharmacological treatments were associated with harms (anorexia, weight loss and insomnia), but an increased risk of serious adverse events was not observed." It should be no surprise to anyone that there is a cost-benefit balance to be struck when it comes to pharmacotherapy for ADHD or anything else. Medicines always have the potential for side-effects. The lack however, of 'serious adverse events' noted by Catalá-López et al is reassuring and adds to other reviews (see here) highlighting a role for regular monitoring and good medicines management with specific regards to medicines indicated for ADHD.

Finally: "There is lack of evidence for cognitive training, neurofeedback, antidepressants, antipsychotics, dietary therapy, fatty acids, and other complementary and alternative medicine." I don't disagree with these findings but do find it a little 'odd' that other systematic reviews/meta-analyses of something like dietary interventions and/or use of fatty acids for ADHD have come to slightly different conclusions (see here and see here respectively). I don't doubt that there are going to be some subtle differences in what studies were included for further analysis and the interpretation of findings, but 'lack of evidence' is, in my mind at least, perhaps not an entirely accurate viewpoint. Indeed, a more recent paper is a further case in point [3].

"An open and honest discussion with parents and older children about uncertainties of available treatments and the balance between benefits, costs, and potential harms should be established before starting treatment." I think a sentence like this should be added to every article trying to arrive at a coherent statement for many different behavioural and psychiatric labels. It tells us that whilst scientific progress is being made when it comes to the important management of ADHD (see here), there typically is no one-size-fits-all 'silver bullet' that will vanquish all of the challenging symptoms of ADHD and onward improve current and future quality of life. Added to the idea that a multi-pronged approach to managing ADHD is typically better than any one single intervention option, and the Catalá-López article might turn out to be something rather important for many different people with various degrees of interest in ADHD and beyond.

Music: and when Mars Attacks...

----------

[1] Catalá-López F. et al. The pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: A systematic review with network meta-analyses of randomised trials. PLoS One. 2017 Jul 12;12(7):e0180355.

[2] Catalá-López F. et al. The pharmacological and non-pharmacological treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: protocol for a systematic review and network meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Systematic Reviews. 2015;4:19.

[3] Chang JC. et al. Omega-3 Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids in Youths with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Clinical Trials and Biological Studies. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017 Jul 25.

----------

Wednesday 2 August 2017

Fidget spinners: "their alleged benefits remain scientifically unfounded"

Fidget spinners, those spinning toys that have taken the world by storm in recent months, are great little things. My brood have several and, at least for the first few days of getting them, were constantly spinning them on their fingers or any other suitable surface. A welcome - if temporary - distraction from the tablets, phones, games consoles et al that pervade their lives.

Some of the claims however, that have been made about fidget spinners are not so great. Namely that these toys might have some almost magical qualities in 'eliminating anxiety' in relation to various diagnostic labels such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or autism. The paper by Rachel Schecter and colleagues [1] confirms however that as things currently stand: "their alleged benefits remain scientifically unfounded."

Accepting that the research literature on the use of fidget spinners is, at the moment, pretty much nil, Schecter et al provide a little peer-reviewed clarity on the various claims being made around fidget spinners and conditions like ADHD and autism. They suggest that because  "fidget spinners and other self-regulatory occupational therapy toys have yet to be subjected to rigorous scientific research" any medicinal claims around them should really be reserved until appropriate scientific evidence emerges. Further that such toys may represent "potential choking hazards" so medical professionals (and I assume parents and caregivers also) need to be mindful. Such findings follow other opinions from members of this authorship group on other modern day trends and fads [2].

I don't want to undermine the popularity and usefulness of something like fidget spinners as a toy. I can remember the hacky sack sharing a similarly popular position when I was a kid; albeit not accompanied by any psychobabble explanations of improved this-or-that accompanying such use. I neither want to totally poo-poo the idea that for some at least, fidget spinners *might* offer some relief for something like stress under certain circumstances. But what I will continue to take exception to is the way that loose words about a toy having some medicinal qualities for one or more groups can seemingly fill the marketing airways without rigorous scientific evidence to back them up...

To close, vicars... careful when you go to the pub.

----------

[1] Schecter RA. et al. Fidget spinners: Purported benefits, adverse effects and accepted alternatives. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2017 Jul 7.

[2] Serino M. et al. Pokémon Go and augmented virtual reality games: a cautionary commentary for parents and pediatricians. Curr Opin Pediatr. 2016 Oct;28(5):673-7.

----------